首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
外语
Preschoolers’ Innate Knowledge Means They Can Probably Do Algebra Child development specialists are uncovering evidence that
Preschoolers’ Innate Knowledge Means They Can Probably Do Algebra Child development specialists are uncovering evidence that
admin
2015-03-28
75
问题
Preschoolers’ Innate Knowledge Means They Can Probably Do Algebra
Child development specialists are uncovering evidence that toddlers may understand much more than we think.
[A] Give a three-year old a smart phone and she’ll likely figure out how to turn it on and operate a few simple functions. But confront her with an algebra problem and ask her to solve for x? Not likely.
[B] For decades, child developmental psychologist Jean Piaget convinced us that young, undeveloped minds couldn’t handle complex concepts because they simply weren’t experienced or mature enough yet. Piaget, in fact, believed that toddlers could not understand cause and effect, that they couldn’t think logically, and that they also couldn’t handle abstract ideas.
[C] That’s because, he argued, children learn to develop these higher skills through trial and error. But child development specialists are finding out that preschoolers without any formal education may have the capacity to understand more complex concepts than we give them credit for, such as complicated rules for operating a toy or even solving for an unknown in algebra. Some of this is due to their ability to be more open and flexible about their world than adults. But beyond that, toddlers may have the innate ability to understand abstract concepts like quantities and causality, and that’s fueling an exciting stream of experiments that reveal just how sophisticated preschoolers’ brains might be.
[D] Alison Gopnik, professor of psychology at University of California Berkeley and her team devised a way to test how well young kids understand the abstract concept of multiple causality — the idea that there may be more than one cause for a single effect. They pitted 32 preschoolers around 4 years old against 143 un-dergrads. The study centered around a toy that could be turned on by placing a single blue-colored block on the toy’s tray, but could also be activated if two blocks of different colors — orange and purple — were placed on the tray. Both the kids and the undergraduates were shown how the toy worked and then asked which blocks activated the toy.
[E] The preschoolers were adept at figuring out that the blue blocks turned on the toy, as did the purple and orange ones. The Berkeley undergraduates, however, had a harder time accepting the scenario.Their previous experience in the world, which tends to work in a single-cause-equals-single-effect way, hampered their ability to accept the unusual rules that activated they toy; they wanted to believe that it was activated either by a single color or by a combination of colors, but not both.
[F] The preschoolers’ lack of bias about causality likely contributed to their ability to learn the multiple ways to activate the toy, but the results also suggest that preschoolers really can think logically and in more complicated ways. Just because they can’t express themselves or aren’t as adept at demonstrating such knowledge, doesn’t mean they don’t have it.
[G] Researchers from Johns Hopkins University, for example, found a similar effect among preschoolers when it came to math. Previous studies showed that if you present infants with eight objects over and over until they got bored, and then showed them 16, they suddenly regained interest and sensed that things changed. "All the evidence so far leads us to believe that this is something that babies come into the world with," says Melissa Kibbe, co-author of that study.
[H] She and her colleague Lisa Feigenson wondered if that innate sense of quantity might translate into an understanding of numbers and higher math functions, including solving for unknowns — one of the foundations of algebra — which often isn’t taught until seventh or eighth grades. So they conducted a series of experiments using a cup with a fixed amount of objects that substituted for x in the equation 5 + x=17.
[I] To divert the four- and six-year olds’ attention away from Arabic numerals to quantities instead, the researchers used a puppet and a "magic" cup that contained 12 buttons. In one of the experiments, the children saw five buttons on the table. After watching the researchers add the 12 buttons from the cup, they were told there were 17 buttons on the table. In another test, the youngsters saw three piles of objects — buttons, coins or small toys — in varying amounts, and observed the researchers adding the fixed number of contents of the puppet’s cup to each.
[J] After training the kids on how the cup worked, the researchers tried to confuse them with another cup containing fewer (such as four) or more (such as 24) objects. However, the kids understood intuitively that the decoy cup contained the wrong amount of items and that a specific amount — x, the "magic" cup amount — had to be added to reach the sum. That suggested that the preschoolers had some concept of quantity. What surprised Kibbe was not just that preschoolers understood the concept of adding "more," but that they could also calibrate how much more was needed to fill in the unknown quantity.
[K] "These kids had very little formal schooling so far, but what we are finding is that when we tap into their gut sense, something we call the Approximate Number Sense (ANS), kids are able to do much more complex calculations than if we gave them numbers and letters," says Kibbe of her results. And there doesn’t seem to be any gender differences in this innate ability, at least not among the girls and boys Kibbe studied.
[L] There’s also precedent for such innate pre-learning in reading, says Jon Star, at the Harvard University Graduate School of Education. To improve reading skills, some teachers have tapped into children’s memorization skills to make the connection between words and meaning more efficient.
[M] Kibbe’s and Gopnik’s recent work may have broader implications for education, since current math curricula in schools, which focuses on teaching Arabic numerals and on solving equations, may not be ideal for nurturing the number sense that kids are born with. "There’s an exciting movement in psychology over the past decade, as we learn that students bring certain capabilities, or innate knowledge that we hadn’t thought they had before," says Star.
[N] Though it may be too early to translate such findings to the classroom, the results lay the groundwork for studying similar innate skills and how they might be better understood. ANS, for example, is one of many constructs that young children may have that could enhance their learning but that current curricula aren’t exploiting. Developmental experts are still trying to figure out how malleable these constructs are, and how much of an impact they can have on future learning. For instance, do kids who hone their ANS skills become better at algebra and calculus in high school? "We still need to figure out which constructs matter most, and which are most amenable to interventions to help children improve their learning," says Star.
[O] "The hard part is, educationally, how do you build up and upon this intuitive knowledge in a way that allows a child to capture the complexity but not hold them back," says Tina Grotzer, associate professor of education at Harvard. Tapping into a child’s still developing sense of numbers and quantities is one thing, but overloading it with too many new constructs about algebra, unknowns, and problem solving may just gum up the working memory and end up adversely affecting his learning and academic performance.
[P] Still, that doesn’t mean that these innate skills shouldn’t be explored and possibly exploited in the class-room. Preschoolers may be smarter than we think, but we still have to figure out how to give them the right opportunities in the classroom so they know what to do with that knowledge.
Current math curricula in schools are not good enough for kids to develop their number sense, because they mainly deal with Arabic numerals and equations.
选项
答案
M
解析
段首句提到,如今学校的数学课程都把重点放在教授阿拉伯数字和求解方程式上面,这可能并不是培养孩子与生俱来的数字观念的理想方式。由此可见,学校的数学课程可能并不是培养孩子与生俱来的数字观念的理想方式,因为它们都把重点放在教授阿拉伯数字和求解方程式上面。本题是对该句的转述。题中的are not good enough对应文中的may not be ideal,develop对应nurturing。
转载请注明原文地址:https://kaotiyun.com/show/Weh7777K
0
大学英语六级
相关试题推荐
ShouldCollegesFocusonTalentTrainingorScientificResearch?Manyoftoday’scollegestudentsaresufferingfromaform
Theidealcompanionmachinewouldnotonlylook,feel,andsoundfriendlybutwouldalsobeprogrammedtobehaveinanagreeable
A、Tocelebratetheirvictory.B、Tocheerupthehunters.C、Toscarethewolves.D、Toalertthedeer.D目的原因题。短文中提到,为了保护白尾鹿,当地的一些动
Cellphonesprovideinstantaccesstopeople.Theyarecreatingamajor【C1】______inthesocialexperiencesofbothchildrenand
北京烤鸭是自封建帝王时代就在北京城流行的著名菜肴,如今它被认为是中国的一道国菜。这道菜以它薄而脆的酥皮,以及厨师们在客人面前片鸭子的真实情形而著称。专门用于制作烤鸭的鸭子在养殖65天后就被屠宰了,鸭子在烤制前要先用调料腌制(season),然后才送进焖炉
筷子是广泛适用于东亚各国的传统餐具。筷子的历史起源于中国古代,最早可以追溯到商代。筷子通常由竹子、塑料、木材或不锈钢制成。第一双筷子可能用于烹饪,拨火而不是作为饮食器具。直到汉代,筷子才开始用作餐具。直到明代,筷子才开始被普遍使用于上菜或者吃饭。然后它们被
A、Heislaidofffromhisjob.B、Heisshortofmoney.C、Thehouseherentsisverylarge.D、Heisverylonely.B对话中女士提到,她过来看看男士
A、Themanandthewomanliveindifferentbuildings.B、Thewomanisupsetthatshewasn’tinvitedtotheparty.C、Thewomanisc
OneofthoserarelocalcreationsofAmerica,cowboypoetryhasalongandvividhistory,drivenbyitscolourfulpractitioners
Mytopicishandedness—whetherindifferentsportsitisbettertobeleftorright-sidedorwhetheramorebalancedapproachis
随机试题
A、医疗用药的重大问题B、基本用药目录和处方手册C、贯彻执行药政法规的情况D、用药计划E、新制剂医疗机构药事管理委员会的任务之一是制(修)订本院
丹参治疮痈肿痛,其作用在于()
中国某公司进口了一批皮制品,信用证方式支付,以海运方式运输并投保了一切险。中国收货人持正本提单提货时发现货物已被他人提走。依相关司法解释和国际惯例,下列哪一选项是正确的?()
(2004)热量传递有三种基本方式,它们是导热、对流和辐射。关于热量传递下面哪个说法是不正确的?
不属于委托人权利的是( )。
《建设工程安全生产管理条例》规定,施工单位的主要负责人、项目负责人违章指挥、强令职工冒险作业,尚不够刑事处罚的,将处()罚款或者按照管理权限给予撤职处分。
影响经济主体对货币需求量的主要因素有()。
不等式组的解在数轴上可以表示为()。
“一把手”应如何处理好与副职的关系?
阅读下列说明。回答问题,将解答填入答题纸的对应栏内。【说明】某单位进行大型电子政务信息系统工程建设,总投资5000万元。建设单位分别与承建单位、监理单位签订了实施合同和监理合同。【事件1】承建单位在合同时间范围内完成了业务应用系统的代码开发工作,将软
最新回复
(
0
)