首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
外语
We all know that we don’t get enough sleep. But how much sleep do we really need? Until about 15 years ago, one common theory wa
We all know that we don’t get enough sleep. But how much sleep do we really need? Until about 15 years ago, one common theory wa
admin
2017-04-20
61
问题
We all know that we don’t get enough sleep. But how much sleep do we really need? Until about 15 years ago, one common theory was that if you slept at least four or five hours a night, your cognitive performance remained intact; your body simply adapted to less sleep. But that idea was based on studies in which researchers sent sleepy subjects home during the day—where they may have sneaked in naps and downed coffee.
Enter David Dinges, the head of the Sleep and Chronobiology Laboratory at the Hospital at University of Pennsylvania, who has the distinction of depriving more people of sleep than perhaps anyone in the world. In what was the longest sleep-restriction study of its kind, Dinges and his lead author, Hans Van Dongen, assigned dozens of subjects to three different groups for their 2003 study: some slept four hours, others six hours and others, for the lucky control group, eight hours— for two weeks in the lab.
Every two hours during the day, the researchers tested the subjects’ ability to sustain attention with what’s known as the psychomotor vigilance task, or P. V. T., considered a gold standard of sleepiness measures. During the P. V. T., the men and women sat in front of computer screens for 10-minute periods, pressing the space bar as soon as they saw a flash of numbers at random intervals. Even a half-second response delay suggests a lapse into sleepiness, known as a microsleep.
The P. V. T. is tedious but simple if you’ve been sleeping well. It measures the sustained attention that is vital for pilots, truck drivers, astronauts. Attention is also key for focusing during long meetings; for reading a paragraph just once, instead of five times; for driving a car. It takes the equivalent of only a two-second lapse for a driver to veer into oncoming traffic.
Not surprisingly, those who had eight hours of sleep hardly had any attention lapses and no cognitive declines over the 14 days of the study. What was interesting was that those in the four-and six-hour groups had P. V. T. results that declined steadily with almost each passing day. Though the four-hour subjects performed far worse, the six-hour group also consistently fell off-task. By the sixth day, 25 percent of the six-hour group was falling asleep at the computer. And at the end of the study, they were lapsing fives times as much as they did the first day. The six-hour subjects fared no better—steadily declining over the two weeks—on a test of working memory in which they had to remember numbers and symbols and substitute one for the other. The same was true for an addition-subtraction task that measures speed and accuracy. All told, by the end of two weeks, the six-hour sleepers were as impaired as those who, in another Dinges study, had been sleep-deprived for 24 hours straight—the cognitive equivalent of being legally drunk.
So, for most of us, eight hours of sleep is excellent and six hours is no good, but what about if we split the difference? What is the threshold below which cognitive function begins to flag? While Dinges’s study was under way, his colleague Gregory Belenky, then director of the division of neuroscience at the Walter Reed Army Institute of Research in Silver Spring, Md. , was running a similar study. He purposely restricted his subjects to odd numbers of sleep hours—three, five, seven and nine hours—so that together the studies would offer a fuller picture of sleep-restriction. Belenky’s nine-hour subjects performed much like Dinges’s eight-hour ones. But in the seven-hour group, their response time on the P. V. T. slowed and continued to do so for three days, before stabilizing at lower levels than when they started. Americans average 6. 9 hours on weeknights, according to the National Sleep Foundation. Which means that, whether we like it or not, we are not thinking as clearly as we could be.
Of course our lives are more stimulating than a sleep lab: we have coffee, bright lights, the social buzz of the office, all of which work as "countermeasures" to sleepiness. They can do the job for only so long, however. As Belenky, who now heads up the Sleep and Performance Research Center at Washington State University, Spokane, where Van Dongen is also a professor, told me about cognitive deficits: "You don’t see it the first day. But you do in five to seven days."
And it’s not clear that we can rely on weekends to make up for sleep deprivation. Dinges is now running a long-term sleep restriction and recovery study to see how many nights we need to erase our sleep debt. But past studies suggest that, at least in many cases, one night alone won’t do it.
Why did Gregory Belenky restrict his subjects to odd numbers of sleep hours?
选项
A、Because he could acquire more precise information.
B、Because he could provide different tests.
C、Because he could find out the utmost effects of sleep.
D、Because he could observe the relationship between sleep and cognition.
答案
A
解析
细节题。根据题干关键词“Gregory Belenky”定位到第六段。该段第四句提到,格雷戈里.贝恩凯为了想知道低于多少小时的睡眠会造成认知功能下降,他故意把他们的睡眠小时数限制在奇数上,如3、5、7、9等,结果他的九小时组受试者表现得与丁格斯八小时组成员不相上下,而七小时组的参与者在接受P.V.T.测试时反应也慢了,这意味着,大部分人的睡眠都不够,思维能力都没有应有的那么清晰。[A]“因为他能得到更确切的信息”与原文一致,为正确答案。[B]“因为他要进行不同的实验”、[C]“因为他能发现睡眠的最大作用”和[D]“因为他能观察到睡眠与认知能力之间的关系”都与这个实验分组的目的不相符,故均可排除。
转载请注明原文地址:https://kaotiyun.com/show/bNzK777K
0
专业英语八级
相关试题推荐
OnaLosAngelesstreetcornerin2000,Iwasthe"insideman"inaclassiccongamecalledthepigeondrop.AmagiciannamedDa
BiancaSforzaattractedfewstareswhenintroducedtotheartworldonJanuary30,1998.Shewasjustaprettyfaceinaframet
SomePremisesunderWhichLinguistsOperateWhenwedolinguisticresearch,weneedtoknowsomeofthepremisesunderwhic
ThereasonswhywomenbossareunpopularincludeallofthefollowingEXCEPTthatpeoplethink______.
Thepurposeof60billiondollarsinvestmentbyFiatisto______.
InterculturalLearningManyteachersmaywonder"whatIamactuallydoing"sometimes.Itdoesn’tseemenoughtoteachgramma
Youarewhatyoueat,orsothesayinggoes.ButRichardWrangham,ofHarvardUniversity,believesthatthisistrueinamorep
Youarewhatyoueat,orsothesayinggoes.ButRichardWrangham,ofHarvardUniversity,believesthatthisistrueinamorep
HighinthemountainsofsouthernFrance,thesleepytownofAurillachasfewobviouscharmstoattracttheoutsider.Iftheset
PASSAGETHREEWhydidn’ttheSephardiccommunitiesinTurkeyandBulgariasufferfromtheNaziHolocaustduringWorldWarII?
随机试题
迈克尔·波特教授把竞争战略划分为()等类型。
每逢经前或经期,巅顶头痛,烦躁易怒,口苦咽干,其辨证为:
某项工作有三项紧后工作,其持续时间分别为4d、5d、6d;其最迟完成时间分别为18d、16d、14d,本工作的最迟完成时间是( )d。
市场风险资本要求涵盖的风险范围包括()。I.全部的外汇风险Ⅱ.全部的商品风险Ⅲ.交易账户中的股票风险Ⅳ.交易账户中的利率风险
异议信息确实有误,但因技术原因无法修改时,征信服务中心应()。
甲是乙公司的研发人员,经长期研究,完成单位交付的研发任务,开发出了一种抗癌新药,现欲申请专利。以下关于该成果权利归属的说法中,正确的有()。
(2008年卷二第34题)根据反不正当竞争法的规定,下列哪些行为属于侵犯商业秘密的行为?
以高压甚至超高压将发电厂、变电站或变电站之间连接起来的送电网络称为()。
下列关于法律调整方法的表述,能够成立的是()。
简单商品生产的基本矛盾是()
最新回复
(
0
)