首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
外语
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it come
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it come
admin
2016-03-10
42
问题
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it comes from God, or else through die application of reason.
But in The Bonobo and the Atheist, primatologist Frans de Waal argues that there’s another answer that fits the data better: morality comes from our evolutionary past as a social primate(灵长目动物). Like our closest relatives, the apes, humans evolved in small, tightly knit, cooperative groups. As a result, again like the apes, we are exquisitely sensitive to one another’s moods, needs and intentions.
This well-developed empathy provided the trellis(框架)on which morality later flowered. De Waal, who is based at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, has been making this case eloquently for many years and over several books, notably in Good Natured back in 1997, and in Primates and Philosophers, 12 years later.
In his new work, he bolsters(支持)the argument by drawing on a lot of new research, carefully footnoted for those who want to dig deeper. De Waal distinguishes two degrees of morality. The first he calls "one-on-one morality", which governs how an individual can expect to be treated, and the second "community concern" , a larger, more abstract concept that extends to the harmony of the group as a whole.
Chimps and bonobos certainly have the former—they respect ownership, for example, and expect to be treated according to their place in the hierarchy. But de Waal presents several examples—such as a chimp stepping in to stop a fight between two others—that suggest that they also have a rudimentary(初步的)form of the latter.
The book’s title, incidentally, draws on bonobos because they are more likely than chimps to behave morally, to have concern for each other, to value harmony and so on. This, imagines, de Waal, is something morally inclined atheists would want to emulate. If humans inherited morality from our ancestors, though, what are we to make of religion? Here de Waal moves into the territory he has not explored before. Clearly, religion must do something important, since every human culture has it. But instead of religion giving us morality, de Waal turns the tables. Morality, he argues, probably gave us religion as a way of reinforcing the pre-existing community concern.
If he’s right, then there may be no absolute code of right and wrong out there to be discovered. Instead, each individual’s evolved sense of empathy and concern for the group may help shape the group’s consensus on what kind of behaviour is appropriate. In short, says de Waal, morality may be something we all have to work out together. It’s a persuasive argument, and de Waal’s cautious and evidence-based approach is one that many New Scientist readers are sure to find congenial.
That careful approach is less evident in another book covering some of the same ground. In How Animals Grieve, anthropologist Barbara King sets out to explore the question of whether non-human animals grieve for their dead. It’s an intriguing question, but unfortunately King’s book is largely a succession of anecdotes: the cat who roams the house, crying, in search of its dead litter mate: the dog who waits daily at the train station for its dead master: a dolphin trying to keep her dead calf afloat for days.
Some of these stories make a persuasive case for some animals—especially apes, elephants and cetaceans— sometimes grieving. No surprises there: I suspect most readers would have conceded that ground right from the start.
But King makes little effort to dig any deeper by exploring, for example, the neural machinery and cognitive skills an animal needs in order to be capable of grief. After all, solitary species such as cats have less need for empathy—and its corollary, grief—than social animals, and small-brained creatures such as turtles may simply lack the brainpower or not form lasting pair bonds.
To his credit, de Waal takes full note of such distinctions: King, not so much.
Which statement would Frans de Waal most probably agree to?
选项
A、Bonobos are not as concerned about each other as chimps.
B、Atheists would like to emulate the harmony among Bonobos.
C、Bonobos are a good example to show where morality comes from.
D、Atheists are a group of people who are lack of moral standards.
答案
C
解析
细节题。作者在第六段第一句提到Frans de Waal的书时指出,书名用倭黑猩猩(bonobos)是因为它们比黑猩猩(chimps)在行为上表现得更道德,可见Frans de Waal认为,倭黑猩猩是说明道德来源的好例子,故[C]为答案。
转载请注明原文地址:https://kaotiyun.com/show/mb7O777K
0
专业英语八级
相关试题推荐
Accordingtotheinterviewee,whatistheproblemofthepresentscienceeducation?
NewstandardsforfilialpietyreleasedbytheChinesegovernmentspecifying24requirementsofdutifulchildrentriggeredahea
Itcouldbearguedthathumanbeingsareaspeciesofanimal.However,theuniquecharacteristics,whichmakesmankinddifferent
AfterTheAdventuresofTomSawyer,TwaingivesaliteraryindependencetoTom’sbuddyHuckinabookentitled______
Thefactthatwecanalwayswritenewsentencestoexpressournewideasisagoodillustrationofthe______featureoflanguage.
Kissingissocommonthoughwerarelyaskwhyhumanstouch【M1】______theirlipstogethertoshowaffection.Oneobviousanswer
Kissingissocommonthoughwerarelyaskwhyhumanstouch【M1】______theirlipstogethertoshowaffection.Oneobviousanswer
Kissingissocommonthoughwerarelyaskwhyhumanstouch【M1】______theirlipstogethertoshowaffection.Oneobviousanswer
TheSeattleTimesCompanyisonenewspaperfirmthathasrecognizedtheneedforchangeanddonesomethingaboutit.Inthene
随机试题
女,45岁。半年来剧烈活动时诱发胸骨后疼痛,休息数分钟可自行缓解,近5天来发作频繁,且于上楼或步行时均可诱发,夜间也有发作。BP130/80mmHg,P63次/分,该患者的最佳治疗方案是()
小儿出生时即具有一些先天性反射,不属于先天性反射的是
下列级别中,不属于全国公路建设从业单位信用评价等级的是()。[2013年真题]
2009年7月,A国有企业欲以协议收购方式收购C上市公司。具体作法为:A企业与C公司的发起人股东D国有企业订立协议,受让D企业持有的C公司51%的股份。在收购协议订立之前,C公司必须召开股东大会通过此事。在收购协议订立之后,D企业必须在3日内将该收购协议报
股体语言在日常生活中无处不在,其作用包括()
2018年2月7日,我国第五个南极科考站——()在恩克斯堡岛正式选址奠基。
阶级社会中占统治地位的社会意识是()。
紫禁城北端的御花园是一座典型的宫廷式花园,在1.2万平方米的有限空间,容纳了大小20多座亭、台、楼、阁,其建筑形式采用与宫殿建筑相和谐的统一对称的手法,又富于高低,大小的变化,整个花园既保持了宫殿的规整,又不失园林的活泼。从这段话中可以概括出紫禁城御花园的
在实际操作系统中,一般采用(14)排除死锁现象。
Wheredoesthisconversationmostprobablytakeplace?
最新回复
(
0
)