首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
外语
READING PASSAGE 3 You should spend about 20 minutes on Questions 28-40 which are based on Reading Passage 3 below
READING PASSAGE 3 You should spend about 20 minutes on Questions 28-40 which are based on Reading Passage 3 below
admin
2009-05-13
63
问题
READING PASSAGE 3
You should spend about 20 minutes on Questions 28-40 which are based on Reading Passage 3 below
Striking the RIGHT NOTE
Is perfect a rare talent possessed solely by the likes of Beethoven?
Kathryn Brown discusses this much sought-after musical ability
The uncanny, if sometimes distracting, ability to name a solitary note out of the blue, without any other notes for reference, is a prized musical talent- and a scientific mystery. Musicians with perfect pitch - or, as many researchers prefer to call it, absolute pitch - can often play pieces by ear, and many can transcribe music brilliantly. That’s because they perceive the position of a note in the musical stave - its pitch - as clearly as the fact that they heard it. Hearing and naming the pitch go hand in hand.
By contrast, most musicians follow not the notes, but the relationship between them. They may easily recognise two notes as being a certain number of tones apart, but could name the higher note as an E only if they are told the lower one is a C, for example. This is relative pitch. Useful, but much less mysterious.
For centuries, absolute pitch has been thought of as the preserve of the musical elite. Some estimates suggest that maybe fewer than 1 in 2,000 people possess it. But a growing number of studies, from speech experiments to brain scans, are now suggesting that a knack for absolute pitch may be far more common, and more varied, than previously thought. ’Absolute pitch is not an all or nothing feature,’ says Marvin, a music theorist at the University of Rochester in New York state. Some researchers even claim that we could all develop the skill, regardless of our musical talent. And their work may finally settle a decades-old debate about whether absolute pitch depends on melodious genes - or early music lessons.
Music psychologist Diana Deutsch at the University of California in San Diego is the leading voice. Last month at the Acoustical Society of America meeting in Columbus, Ohio, Deutsch reported a study that suggests we all have the potential to acquire absolute pitch - and that speakers of tone languages use it every day. A third of the world’s population - chiefly people in Asia and Africa - speak tone languages, in which a word’s meaning can vary depending on the pitch a speaker uses.
Deutsch and her colleagues asked seven native Vietnamese speakers and 15 native Mandarin speakers to read out lists of words on different days. The chosen words spanned a range of pitches, to force the speakers to raise and lower their voices considerably. By recording these recited lists and taking the average pitch for each whole word, the researchers compared the pitches used by each person to say each word on different days.
Both groups showed strikingly consistent pitch for any given word - often less than a quarter-tone difference between days. ’The similarity,’ Deutsch says, ’is mind-boggling.’ It’s also, she says, a real example of absolute pitch. As babies, the speakers learnt to associate certain pitches with meaningful words - just as a musician labels one tone A and another B - and they demonstrate this precise use of pitch regardless of whether or not they have had any musical training, she adds.
Deutsch isn’t the only researcher turning up everyday evidence of absolute pitch. At least three other experiments have found that people can launch into familiar songs at or very near the correct pitches. Some researchers have nicknamed this ability ’absolute memory’, and they say it pops up on other senses, too. Given studies like these, the real mystery is why we don’t all have absolute pitch, says cognitive psychologist Daniel Levitin of McGill University in Montreal.
Over the past decade, researchers have confirmed that absolute pitch often runs in families. Nelson Freimer of the University of California in San Francisco, for example, is just completing a study that he says strongly suggests the right genes help create this brand of musical genius. Freimer gave tone tests to people with absolute pitch and to their relatives. He also tested several hundred other people who had taken early music lessons. He found that relatives of people with absolute pitch were far more likely to develop the skill than people who simply had the music lessons. ’There is clearly a familial aggregation of absolute pitch,’ Freimer says.
Freimer says some children are probably genetically predisposed toward absolute pitch - and this innate inclination blossoms during childhood music lessons. Indeed, many researchers now point to this harmony of nature and nurture to explain why musicians with absolute pitch show different levels of the talent.
Indeed, researchers are finding more and more evidence suggesting music lessons are critical to the development of absolute pitch. In a survey of 2,700 students in American music conservatories and college programmes, New York University geneticist Peter Gregersen and his colleagues found that a whopping 32 per cent of the Asian students reported having absolute pitch, compared with just 7 per cent of non-Asian students. While that might suggest a genetic tendency towards absolute pitch in the Asian population, Gregersen says that the type and timing of music lessons probably explains much of the difference.
For one thing, those with absolute pitch started lessons, on average, when they were five years old, while those without absolute pitch started around the age of eight. Moreover, adds Gregersen, the type of music lessons favoured in Asia, and by many of the Asian families in his study, such as the Suzuki method, often focus on playing by ear and learning the names of musical notes, while those more commonly used in the US tend to emphasise learning scales in a relative pitch way. In Japanese pre-school music programmes, he says, children often have to listen to notes played on a piano and hold up a coloured flag to signal the pitch. ’There’s a distinct cultural difference,’ he says.
Deutsch predicts that further studies will reveal absolute pitch - in its imperfect, latent form - inside all of us. The Western emphasis on relative pitch simply obscures it, she contends. ’It’s very likely that scientists will end up concluding that we’re all born with the potential to acquire very fine-grained absolute pitch. It’s really just a matter of life getting in the way.’
选项
答案
ability
解析
First line of text
转载请注明原文地址:https://kaotiyun.com/show/weVO777K
本试题收录于:
雅思阅读题库雅思(IELTS)分类
0
雅思阅读
雅思(IELTS)
相关试题推荐
Anewhandhelddevicepurportstodeterminetheseverityofconcussionsbyreadingthebrain’selectricalsignalsandcomparing
BeforeColette,thefemalewritersofFrancehadbeenaristocrats,fromMmedeLafayettetoAnnedeNoailles;therewerenoJane
ThefollowingappearedinahealthmagazinepublishedinCorpora."Medicalexpertssaythatonlyone-quarterofCorpora’scitize
Thefollowingappearedinamemofromthedirectorofalargegroupofhospitals."Inalaboratorystudyofliquidantibacterial
Claim:Whileboredomisoftenexpressedwithasenseofself-satisfaction,itshouldreallybeasourceofembarrassment.Reason
Allparentsshouldberequiredtovolunteertimetotheirchildren’sschools.Writearesponseinwhichyoudiscusstheextentt
DirectionsforQuantitativeComparisonQuestions:Someofthefollowingquestionsgiveyoutwoquantities,oneinColumnAando
DirectionsforQuantitativeComparisonQuestions:Someofthefollowingquestionsgiveyoutwoquantities,oneinColumnAando
随机试题
下列关于接入技术特征的描述中,错误的是()。
列宁关于社会主义建设理论的主要贡献不包括()
核酶的特点
分析技术方案盈利能力的指标包括方案财务内部收益率和财务净现值、资本金财务内部收益率、静态投资回收期,以及()。
某企业在某经营周期内发生营业支出10万元,营业外支出5万元,管理费用2万元,财务费用1.5万元,获捐价值1万元的办公设备。则其期间费用应以()万元计。
企业债权投资获得的利息收入属于让渡资产使用权收入。()
甲的工作单位在H省A市,在B市新开设一个分支机构,因此调甲到B市发展业务。甲因此转让处于A市的一栋住宅给乙,乙现居住在C市。两人在房屋买卖中产生了纠纷提起诉讼,对该案件享有管辖权的法院是()。
五力模型主要是用于战略分析中的()。
A股份有限公司(以下简称A公司)和B股份有限公司(以下简称B公司)均为增值税一般纳税人,适用的增值税税率均为17%。(2000年)1998年1月5日,A公司向B公司销售材料一批,增值税专用发票上注明的价款为500万元,增值税额为85万元。至1999年9月
下列关于海洋灾害的说法,不正确的一项是()。
最新回复
(
0
)