首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
外语
Choice blindness: You don’t know what you want We have all heard of experts who fail basic tests of sensory discrimination i
Choice blindness: You don’t know what you want We have all heard of experts who fail basic tests of sensory discrimination i
admin
2013-07-30
50
问题
Choice blindness: You don’t know what you want
We have all heard of experts who fail basic tests of sensory discrimination in their own field: wine snobs(自命不凡的人)who can’t tell red from white wine(though in blackened cups), or art critics who see deep meaning in random lines drawn by a computer. We delight in such stories since anyone claiming to be an authority is fair game. But what if we shine the spotlight on choices we make about everyday things? Experts might be forgiven for being wrong about the limits of their skills as experts, but could we be forgiven for being wrong about the limits of our skills as experts on ourselves?
We have been trying to answer this question using techniques from magic performances. Rather than playing tricks with alternatives presented to participants, we secretly altered the outcomes of their choices, and recorded how they react. For example, in an early study we showed our volunteers pairs of pictures of faces and asked them to choose the most attractive. In some trials, immediately after they made their choice, we asked people to explain the reasons behind their choices.
Unknown to them, we sometimes used a double-card magic trick to secretly exchange one face for the other so they ended up with the face they did not choose. Common sense dictates that all of us would notice such a big change in the outcome of a choice. But the result showed that in75 per cent of the trials our participants were blind to the mismatch, even offering "reasons" for their "choice".
We called this effect "choice blindness", echoing change blindness, the phenomenon identified by psychologists where a remarkably large number of people fail to spot a major change in their environment. Recall the famous experiments where X asks Y for directions; while Y is struggling to help, X is switched for Z — and Y fails to notice. Researchers are still pondering the full implications, but it does show how little information we use in daily life, and undermines the idea that we know what is going on around us.
When we set out, we aimed to weigh in on the enduring, complicated debate about self-knowledge and intentionality. For all the intimate familiarity we feel we have with decisionmaking, it is very difficult to know about it from the "inside": one of the great barriers for scientific research is the nature of subjectivity.
As anyone who has ever been in a verbal disagreement can prove, people tend to give elaborate justifications for their decisions, which we have every reason to believe are nothing more than rationalisations(文过饰非)after the event. To prove such people wrong, though, or even provide enough evidence to change their mind, is an entirely different matter: who are you to say what my reasons are?
But with choice blindness we drive a large wedge between intentions and actions in the mind. As our participants give us verbal explanations about choices they never made, we can show them beyond doubt — and prove it — that what they say cannot be true. So our experiments offer a unique window into confabulation(虚构)(the story-telling we do to justify things after the fact)that is otherwise very difficult to come by. We can compare everyday explanations with those under lab conditions, looking for such things as the amount of detail in descriptions, how coherent the narrative is, the emotional tone, or even the timing or flow of the speech. Then we can create a theoretical framework to analyse any kind of exchange.
This framework could provide a clinical use for choice blindness: for example, two of our ongoing studies examine how malingering(装病)might develop into true symptoms, and how confabulation might play a role in obsessive-compulsive disorder(强迫症).
Importantly, the effects of choice blindness go beyond snap judgments. Depending on what our volunteers say in response to the mismatched outcomes of choices(whether they give short or long explanations, give numerical rating or labelling, and so on)we found this interaction could change their future preferences to the extent that they come to prefer the previously rejected alternative. This gives us a rare glimpse into the complicated dynamics of self-feedback("I chose this, I publicly said so, therefore I must like it"), which we suspect lies behind the formation of many everyday preferences.
We also want to explore the boundaries of choice blindness. Of course, it will be limited by choices we know to be of great importance in everyday life. Which bride or bridegroom would fail to notice if someone switched their partner at the altar through amazing sleight of hand(巧妙的手段)? Yet there is ample territory between the absurd idea of spouse-swapping, and the results of our early face experiments.
For example, in one recent study we invited supermarket customers to choose between two paired varieties of jam and tea. In order to switch each participant’s choice without them noticing, we created two sets of "magical" jars, with lids at both ends and a divider inside. The jars looked normal, but were designed to hold one variety of jam or tea at each end, and could easily be flipped over.
Immediately after the participants chose, we asked them to taste their choice again and tell us verbally why they made that choice. Before they did, we turned over the sample containers, so the tasters were given the opposite of what they had intended in their selection. Strikingly, people detected no more than a third of all these trick trials. Even when we switched such remarkably different flavors as spicy cinnamon and apple for bitter grapefruit jam, the participants spotted less than half of all switches.
We have also documented this kind of effect when we simulate online shopping for consumer products such as laptops or cellphones, and even apartments. Our latest tests are exploring moral and political decisions, a domain where reflection and deliberation are supposed to play a central role, but which we believe is perfectly suited to investigating using choice blindness.
Throughout our experiments, as well as registering whether our volunteers noticed that they had been presented with the alternative they did not choose, we also quizzed them about their beliefs about their decision processes. How did they think they would feel if they had been exposed to a study like ours? Did they think they would have noticed the switches? Consistently, between 80 and 90 per cent of people said that they believed they would have noticed something was wrong.
Imagine their surprise, even disbelief, when we told them about the nature of the experiments. In everyday decision-making we do see ourselves as knowing a lot about our selves, but like the wine buff or art critic, we often overstate what we know. The good news is that this form of decision snobbery should not be too difficult to treat. Indeed, after reading this article you might already be cured.
Change blindness refers to the phenomenon that______.
选项
A、many people fail to notice the big change around them
B、people tend to ignore the small changes in the surroundings
C、people’s choices can be easily interrupted by a big change
D、quite a few people do not have a good sense of directions
答案
A
解析
该句提到,我们把这种现象称为“选择的盲点”,与“变化的盲点”相仿,后者是心理学家确认到的一个现象,指相当数量的人未能发现他们所在的环境中发生的某个大的变化。[A]many people fail to notice the big change around them是对文中修饰the phenomenon的定语从句where a remark ably large number of people fail to spot a major change in their environment的同义转述,而the phenomenon是对change blindness进行解释说明的同位语,故答案为[A]。[D]项是根据文中提到的问路的例子设的干扰项。
转载请注明原文地址:https://kaotiyun.com/show/PT97777K
0
大学英语六级
相关试题推荐
Directions:Forthispart,youareallowed30minutestowriteashortessayentitledRaisingDogsintheCity.Youshouldwrite
Mostworthwhilecareersrequiresomekindofspecializedtraining.Ideally,therefore,thechoiceofan【C1】______shouldbemade
Opponentsofbilingualeducationtellusthatthepublicisagainstbilingualeducation.Thisimpressionisaresultoftheway
A、Shefeelshervaluewasn’tappreciated.B、Shewantsamoreinterestingjob.C、She’stiredofworking.D、Shewantsmoremoney.
Afewyearsagoitwasfashionabletospeakofagenerationgap,adivisionbetweenyoungpeopleandtheirelders.Parentscompl
A、Invarioustents.B、Inuniversitybuildings.C、Inahospital.D、Inanauditorium.A原文出现的theuniversityathleticfield并没有在选项中出现
A、She’sgoingtoearnmoremoney.B、Sheiswritingabookonliterature.C、Shehasmetsomeinterestingstudents.D、Sheisenjo
Whenshoppingonline,itisimportanttobearafewbasicsinmind.Notonlywillthesehelpkeepyousafebuttheywillalso【B1
A、Stressmaycauseseriousproblems.B、Therearemanymethodsofcopingwithstress.C、Whatisthemostimportantisthatpeople
MyViewonOnlineEducation1.目前网络教育很普及2.你认为网络教育普及的原因3.你的看法
随机试题
Therehebought______chocolateforhisdaughter,andthenhehad______beersinthebarnotfarfromtheschool.
可用于尿崩症的药物有:
患者28岁,HBsAg阳性,住院2天后,顺产一男婴
A、耐用性B、定量限C、检测限D、精密度E、准确度以信噪比10:1确定的方法评价的效能指标为( )。
高龄对吸入麻醉药的MAC值的影响是
打开报表平台,设置报表格式并保存文件。打开考生文件夹(D:\Exam\会计电算化\王某\2017\)下类“利润表—模板.srp”报表,设置B列的列宽为83。
域控制器存储了域内的账户、密码和属于这个域的计算机三项信息。当计算机接入网络时,域控制器首先要鉴别这台计算机是否属于这个域,用户使用的登录账户是否存在,密码是否正确。如果三项信息均正确,则允许登录;如果以上信息有一项不正确,那么域控制器就会拒绝这个用户从这
下列选项中,符合所给图形的变化规律的是:
以下代码的输出结果是()。a=5.2b=2.5print(a//b)
下面是面向对象主要特征之一的是
最新回复
(
0
)