首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
外语
Why Should We Worry About What We Shouldn’t? It would be a lot easier to enjoy your life if there weren’t so many things try
Why Should We Worry About What We Shouldn’t? It would be a lot easier to enjoy your life if there weren’t so many things try
admin
2010-05-09
45
问题
Why Should We Worry About What We Shouldn’t?
It would be a lot easier to enjoy your life if there weren’t so many things trying to kill you every day. The problems start even before you’re fully awake. There’s the fall out of bed that kills 600 Americans each year. There’s the early-morning heart attack, which is 40% more common than those that strike later in the day. There’s the fatal plunge down the stairs, the bite of sausage that gets lodged in your throat, the tumble on the slippery sidewalk as you leave the house, the high-speed automotive pinball game that is your daily commute.
Other dangers stalk you all day long. Will a cabbie’s brakes fail when you’re in the crosswalk? Will you have a violent reaction to bad food? And what about the risks you carry with you all your life? The father and grandfather who died of coronaries in their 50s probably passed the same cardiac weakness on to you. The tendency to take chances on the highway that has twice landed you in traffic court could just as easily land you in the morgue.
Shadowed by peril as we are, you would think we’d get pretty good at distinguishing the risks likeliest to do us in from the ones that are statistical long shots. But you would be wrong. We agonize over avian flu, which to date has killed precisely no one in the U.S., but have to be cajoled into getting vaccinated for the common flu, which contributes to the deaths of 36,000 Americans each year. We wring our hands over the mad cow pathogen that might be (but almost certainly isn’t) in our hamburger and worry far less about the cholesterol that contributes to the heart disease that kills 700,000 of us annually.
We pride ourselves on being the only species that understands the concept of risk, yet we have a confounding habit of worrying about mere possibilities while ignoring probabilities, building barricades against perceived dangers while leaving ourselves exposed to real ones. Six Muslims traveling from a religious conference were thrown off a plane last week in Minneapolis, Minn., even as unscreened cargo continues to stream into ports on both coasts. Shoppers still look askance at a bag of spinach for fear of E. coli bacteria while filling their carts with fat-sodden French fries and salt-crusted nachos. We put filters on faucets, install air ionizers in our homes and lather ourselves with antibacterial soap. "We used to measure contaminants down to the parts per million," says Dan McGinn, a former Capitol Hill staff member and now a private risk consultant. "Now it’s parts per billion."
At the same time, 20% of all adults still smoke; nearly 20% of drivers and more than 30% of backseat passengers don’t use seat belts; two-thirds of us are overweight or obese. We dash across the street against the light and build our homes in hurricane-prone areas and when they’re demolished by a storm, we rebuild in the same spot. Sensible calculation of real-world risks is a multidimensional math problem that sometimes seems entirely beyond us. And while it may be tree that it’s something we’ll never do exceptionally well, it’s almost certainly something we can learn to do better.
Part of the problem we have with evaluating risk, scientists say, is that we’re moving through the modem world with what is, in many respects, a prehistoric brain. We may think we’ve grown accustomed to living in a predator-free environment in which most of the dangers of the wild have been driven away or fenced off, but our central nervous system--evolving at a glacial pace--hasn’t got the message.
To probe the risk-assessment mechanisms of the human mind, Joseph LeDoux, a professor of neuroscience at New York University and the author of The Emotional Brain, studies fear pathways in laboratory animals. He explains that the jumpiest part of the brain--of mouse and man--is the amygdala, a primitive, almond-shaped clump of tissue that sits just above the brainstem. When you spot potential danger--a stick in the grass that may be a snake, a shadow around a comer that could be a mugger--it’s the amygdala that reacts the most dramatically, triggering the fight-or-flight reaction that pumps adrenaline and other hormones into your bloodstream.
It’s not until a fraction of a second later that the higher regions of the brain get the signal and begin to sort out whether the danger is real. But that fraction of a second causes us to experience the fear far more vividly than we do the rational response an advantage that doesn’t disappear with time. The brain is wired in such a way that nerve signals travel more readily from the amygdala to the upper regions than from the upper regions back down. Setting off your internal alarm is quite easy, but shutting it down takes some doing.
"There are two systems for analyzing risk: an automatic, intuitive system and a more thoughtful analysis," says Paul Slovic, professor of psychology at the University of Oregon. "Our perception of risk lives largely in our feelings, so most of the time we’re operating on system No. 1."
There’s clearly an evolutionary advantage to this natural timorousness. If we’re mindful of real dangers and flee when they arise, we’re more likely to live long enough to pass on our genes. But evolutionary rewards also come to those who stand and fight, those willing to take risks--and even suffer injury-- in pursuit of prey or a mate. Our ancestors hunted mastodons and stampeded buffalo, risking getting trampled for the possible payoff of meat and pelt. Males advertised their reproductive fitness by fighting other males, willingly engaging in a contest that could mean death for one and offspring for the other.
These two impulses--to engage danger or turn from it--are constantly at war and have left us with a well-tuned ability to evaluate the costs and payoffs of short-term risk, say Slovic and others. That, however, is not the kind we tend to face in contemporary society, where threats don’t necessarily spring from behind a bush. They’re much more likely to come to us in the form of rumors or news broadcasts or an escalation of the federal terrorism-threat level from orange to red. It’s when the risk and the consequences of our response unfold more slowly, experts say that our analytic system kicks in. This gives us plenty of opportunity to overthink--or underthink-- the problem, and this is where we start to bollix things up. Which risks get excessive attention and which get overlooked depends on a hierarchy of factors. Perhaps the most important is dread. For most creatures, all death is created pretty much equal. Whether you’re eaten by a lion or drowned in a river, your time on the savanna is over. That’s not the way humans see things. The more pain or suffering something causes, the more we tend to fear it; the cleaner or at least quicker the death, the less it troubles us; "We dread anything that poses a greater risk for cancer more than the things that injure us in a traditional way, like an auto crash," says Slovic. "That’s the dread factor." In other words, the more we dread, the more anxious we get, and the more anxious we get, the less precisely we calculate the odds of the thing actually happening. "It’s called probability neglect," says Cass Sunstein, a University of Chicago professor of law specializing in risk regulation.
The same is true for, say, AIDS, which takes you slowly, compared with a heart attack, which can kill you in seconds, despite the fact that heart disease claims nearly 50 times as many Americans than AIDS each year. We also dread catastrophic risks, those that cause the deaths of a lot of people in a single stroke, as opposed to those that kill in a chronic, distributed way.
Unfamiliar threats are similarly scarier than familiar ones. The next E. coli outbreak is unlikely to shake you up as much as the previous one, and any that follow will trouble you even less. In some respects, this is a good thing, particularly if the initial reaction was excessive.
Sensible calculation of real-world risks seems entirely beyond us and it’s something we’ll never do exceptionally well.
选项
A、Y
B、N
C、NG
答案
A
解析
参见第6段4-6行:Sensible calculation of real-world risks is a multidimensional math problem that sometimes seems entirely beyond us.While it may be true that we can never do exceptionally well.大意是:计算现实世界的风险是多领域的数学题,似乎完全超越我们的能力,并且我们永远也不能做得太好。
转载请注明原文地址:https://kaotiyun.com/show/lIKK777K
0
大学英语六级
相关试题推荐
A、Otherlivingthingschangetheirenvironmentwhilemandoesn’t.B、Manaltershisenvironmentwhileheisadaptedtoit,butot
A、Becauseshefeelsveryhotintheroom.B、Becauseshewantstoavoidmeetingpeople.C、Becauseshewantstosmokeacigarette
A、Sleepcanstrengthenmemories.B、Sleepcanrecovermemories.C、Sleepcanprotectmemoriesagainstinterference.D、Sleepcanha
A、Setupyourownwebsite.B、Consultthebank’srepresentatives.C、Trybymakingtransfers.D、Cheekyourstatements.BHowcanon
A、It’snmbythegovernment.B、Itappealstomostpeople.C、Itoffersmuchchoice.D、It’samonopoly.DWhatdoesthemansayabo
A、Tohishome.B、Toherhome.C、Togotothepicnic.D、Togoshopping.AW:Well,theexaminationisovernow,andwecangotot
I’msurealmosteveryoneofyoulookedatyourwatchorataclockbeforeyoucametoclasstoday.Watchesand【B1】______seema
A、Thenoiseinthelibrary.B、Thecrowdedroom.C、Theheatintheoffice.D、Thedisturbedsleep.C语义理解题。女士说;“今天太热了,我都没法工作了。我希望办公
A、Mr.Green.B、Mr.Kelly.C、Mr.Brown.D、Mr.Jim.B
A、It’ssimilartoplayingnotesonapiano.B、Itrequiresanight’ssleep.C、It’ssimilartothewayacomputerstoresinformati
随机试题
在Word中,要输入汉字,插入点指示输入文本将出现的位置,插入点从()移动。
下列选项中,非“是/否”型字段格式的是()
《解放日报》2012年3月23日报道,上海将尝试创办第一所男子学校。提议者、市八中学校长卢起升说,男校模式是在目前普遍出现的男生“学业危机”大背景下应运而生的。名高中男生越来越少,“阴盛阳衰”已是不争的事实。在目前的教育体制与考试制度的背景下,尤其是中小学
试举例说明为什么分析骨髓象必须常规分析血常规。
初期处理火器伤清创后伤口应行一期缝合的是
酶免疫分析的基本技术组成不包括
下列检测消防应急灯具的应急工作时间的方法中,错误的是()。
有3个单位共订300份《人民日报》,每个单位最少订99份,最多101份。问一共有多少种不同的订法?()
从所给四个选项中,选择最合适的一个填入问号处,使之呈现一定规律性:
根据所给资料,回答以下问题。2013年中部地区中单位负责人的年平均工资约比专业技术人员的多:
最新回复
(
0
)