首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
考研
Read the following text and match each of the numbered items in the left column to its corresponding information in the right co
Read the following text and match each of the numbered items in the left column to its corresponding information in the right co
admin
2021-02-21
84
问题
Read the following text and match each of the numbered items in the left column to its corresponding information in the right column. There are two extra choices in the right column. Mark your answers on the ANSWER SHEET. (10 points)
For centuries, philosophers and theologians have almost unanimously held that civilization as we know it depends on a widespread belief in free will—and that losing this belief could be calamitous. Our codes of ethics, for example, assume that we can freely choose between right and wrong. In the Christian tradition, this is known as "moral liberty"—the capacity to discern and pursue the good, instead of merely being compelled by appetites and desires. The great Enlightenment philosopher Immanuel Kant reaffirmed this link between freedom and goodness. If we are not free to choose, he argued, then it would make no sense to say we ought to choose the path of righteousness.
The sciences have grown steadily bolder in their claim that all human behavior can be explained through the clockwork laws of cause and effect. This shift in perception is the continuation of an intellectual revolution that began about 150 years ago, when Charles Darwin first published On the Origin of Species. Shortly after Darwin put forth his theory of evolution, his cousin Sir Francis Galton began to draw out the implications: If we have evolved, then mental faculties like intelligence must be hereditary. But we use those faculties—which some people have to a greater degree than others—to make decisions. So our ability to choose our fate is not free, but depends on our biological inheritance.
Many scientists say that the American physiologist Benjamin Libet demonstrated in the 1980s that we have no free will. It was already known that electrical activity builds up in a person’s brain before she, for example, moves her hand; Libet showed that this buildup occurs before the person consciously makes a decision to move. The conscious experience of deciding to act, which we usually associate with free will, appears to be an add-on, a post hoc reconstruction of events that occurs after the brain has already set the act in motion.
In 2002, two psychologists had a simple but brilliant idea: Instead of speculating about what might happen if people lost belief in their capacity to choose, they could run an experiment to find out. Kathleen Vohs, then at the University of Utah, and Jonathan Schooler, of the University of Pittsburgh, asked one group of participants to read a passage arguing that free will was an illusion, and another group to read a passage that was neutral on the topic. Then they subjected the members of each group to a variety of temptations and observed their behavior. Would differences in abstract philosophical beliefs influence people’s decisions?
Yes, indeed. When asked to take a math test, with cheating made easy, the group primed to see free will as illusory proved more likely to take an illicit peek at the answers. When given an opportunity to steal—to take more money than they were due from an envelope of $ 1 coins—those whose belief in free will had been undermined pilfered more. On a range of measures, Vohs told me, she and Schooler found that "people who are induced to believe less in free will are more likely to behave immorally."
Another pioneer of research into the psychology of free will, Roy Baumeister of Florida State University, has extended these findings. For example, he and colleagues found that students with a weaker belief in free will were less likely to volunteer their time to help a classmate than were those whose belief in free will was stronger. Likewise, those primed to hold a deterministic view by reading statements like "Science has demonstrated that free will is an illusion" were less likely to give money to a homeless person or lend someone a cellphone.
选项
答案
C
解析
根据Immanuel Kant定位到第一段。Immanuel Kant的观点是:If we are not free to choose,then it would make no sense to say we ought to choose the path of righteousness(假如我们不能自由地进行选择,那么谈论应该选择正确道路便失去了意义)。其中reassure和该段倒数第二句中的reaffirm意思相似,故C项为正确选项。
转载请注明原文地址:https://kaotiyun.com/show/JGY4777K
0
考研英语二
相关试题推荐
Britain’sCabinetOfficereleasedasweepingreportonthecountry’sfoodpolicy,anddeterminedthatBritonsarewastingtoomu
Mosthumanbeingsactuallydecidebeforetheythink.Whenanyhumanbeing—executive,specializedexpert,orpersoninthestreet
WetendtothinkofthedecadesimmediatelyfollowingWorldWarIIasatimeofprosperityandgrowth,withsoldiersreturningh
High-qualitycustomerserviceispreachedbymany,butactuallykeepingcustomershappyiseasiersaidthandoneShopperssel
KimiyukiSudashouldbeaperfectcustomerforJapan’scar-makers.He’sayoung,successfulexecutiveatanInternet-servicesco
Theclean-energybusinessisturningintothenextbiginvestmentboom,inwhichrisksarelightlyignored.Untilrecently,reca
RobertaGordonneverthoughtshe’dstillbealiveatage76.Shedefinitelydidn’tthinkshe’dstillbeworking.ButeverySatur
Peopleusedtothinkthatlearningtwolanguagescreatedconfusioninthemind.Far【C1】________itwasthought,togetone
Peopleusedtothinkthatlearningtwolanguagescreatedconfusioninthemind.Far【C1】________itwasthought,togetone
"TheUSeconomyisrapidlydeteriorating,"saysMr.Grannis."Theoddsofarecessionarenowveryhigh,perhapsbytheendoft
随机试题
19世纪60年代,清朝统治集团中倡导洋务的首领人物是()
道德发展的总体趋势是()
______ajobtodo,youandIcouldliveahappylife.
骨髓增生异常综合征最常见的临床表现是
血站违反献血法规定,向医疗机构提供不符合国家规定标准的血液的,应当医疗机构的医务人员违反献血法规定,将不符合国家规定标准的血液用于患者的应当
A.细动脉玻璃样变性B.细动脉纤维化C.细动脉淀粉样变性D.细动脉纤维素样坏死E.细动脉纤维素样变性良性高血压的基本病变是
下列各级数中发散的是()。
电阻电路中,有功功率等于()。
证券营业部的客户服务主要包括()
Themarketinvestigationisindispensabletosalespromotion.Theyareascloselyrelatedasthelipsandteeth.Whatyouproduc
最新回复
(
0
)