首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
外语
Preschoolers’ Innate Knowledge Means They Can Probably Do Algebra Child development specialists are uncovering evidence that
Preschoolers’ Innate Knowledge Means They Can Probably Do Algebra Child development specialists are uncovering evidence that
admin
2015-03-28
57
问题
Preschoolers’ Innate Knowledge Means They Can Probably Do Algebra
Child development specialists are uncovering evidence that toddlers may understand much more than we think.
[A] Give a three-year old a smart phone and she’ll likely figure out how to turn it on and operate a few simple functions. But confront her with an algebra problem and ask her to solve for x? Not likely.
[B] For decades, child developmental psychologist Jean Piaget convinced us that young, undeveloped minds couldn’t handle complex concepts because they simply weren’t experienced or mature enough yet. Piaget, in fact, believed that toddlers could not understand cause and effect, that they couldn’t think logically, and that they also couldn’t handle abstract ideas.
[C] That’s because, he argued, children learn to develop these higher skills through trial and error. But child development specialists are finding out that preschoolers without any formal education may have the capacity to understand more complex concepts than we give them credit for, such as complicated rules for operating a toy or even solving for an unknown in algebra. Some of this is due to their ability to be more open and flexible about their world than adults. But beyond that, toddlers may have the innate ability to understand abstract concepts like quantities and causality, and that’s fueling an exciting stream of experiments that reveal just how sophisticated preschoolers’ brains might be.
[D] Alison Gopnik, professor of psychology at University of California Berkeley and her team devised a way to test how well young kids understand the abstract concept of multiple causality — the idea that there may be more than one cause for a single effect. They pitted 32 preschoolers around 4 years old against 143 un-dergrads. The study centered around a toy that could be turned on by placing a single blue-colored block on the toy’s tray, but could also be activated if two blocks of different colors — orange and purple — were placed on the tray. Both the kids and the undergraduates were shown how the toy worked and then asked which blocks activated the toy.
[E] The preschoolers were adept at figuring out that the blue blocks turned on the toy, as did the purple and orange ones. The Berkeley undergraduates, however, had a harder time accepting the scenario.Their previous experience in the world, which tends to work in a single-cause-equals-single-effect way, hampered their ability to accept the unusual rules that activated they toy; they wanted to believe that it was activated either by a single color or by a combination of colors, but not both.
[F] The preschoolers’ lack of bias about causality likely contributed to their ability to learn the multiple ways to activate the toy, but the results also suggest that preschoolers really can think logically and in more complicated ways. Just because they can’t express themselves or aren’t as adept at demonstrating such knowledge, doesn’t mean they don’t have it.
[G] Researchers from Johns Hopkins University, for example, found a similar effect among preschoolers when it came to math. Previous studies showed that if you present infants with eight objects over and over until they got bored, and then showed them 16, they suddenly regained interest and sensed that things changed. "All the evidence so far leads us to believe that this is something that babies come into the world with," says Melissa Kibbe, co-author of that study.
[H] She and her colleague Lisa Feigenson wondered if that innate sense of quantity might translate into an understanding of numbers and higher math functions, including solving for unknowns — one of the foundations of algebra — which often isn’t taught until seventh or eighth grades. So they conducted a series of experiments using a cup with a fixed amount of objects that substituted for x in the equation 5 + x=17.
[I] To divert the four- and six-year olds’ attention away from Arabic numerals to quantities instead, the researchers used a puppet and a "magic" cup that contained 12 buttons. In one of the experiments, the children saw five buttons on the table. After watching the researchers add the 12 buttons from the cup, they were told there were 17 buttons on the table. In another test, the youngsters saw three piles of objects — buttons, coins or small toys — in varying amounts, and observed the researchers adding the fixed number of contents of the puppet’s cup to each.
[J] After training the kids on how the cup worked, the researchers tried to confuse them with another cup containing fewer (such as four) or more (such as 24) objects. However, the kids understood intuitively that the decoy cup contained the wrong amount of items and that a specific amount — x, the "magic" cup amount — had to be added to reach the sum. That suggested that the preschoolers had some concept of quantity. What surprised Kibbe was not just that preschoolers understood the concept of adding "more," but that they could also calibrate how much more was needed to fill in the unknown quantity.
[K] "These kids had very little formal schooling so far, but what we are finding is that when we tap into their gut sense, something we call the Approximate Number Sense (ANS), kids are able to do much more complex calculations than if we gave them numbers and letters," says Kibbe of her results. And there doesn’t seem to be any gender differences in this innate ability, at least not among the girls and boys Kibbe studied.
[L] There’s also precedent for such innate pre-learning in reading, says Jon Star, at the Harvard University Graduate School of Education. To improve reading skills, some teachers have tapped into children’s memorization skills to make the connection between words and meaning more efficient.
[M] Kibbe’s and Gopnik’s recent work may have broader implications for education, since current math curricula in schools, which focuses on teaching Arabic numerals and on solving equations, may not be ideal for nurturing the number sense that kids are born with. "There’s an exciting movement in psychology over the past decade, as we learn that students bring certain capabilities, or innate knowledge that we hadn’t thought they had before," says Star.
[N] Though it may be too early to translate such findings to the classroom, the results lay the groundwork for studying similar innate skills and how they might be better understood. ANS, for example, is one of many constructs that young children may have that could enhance their learning but that current curricula aren’t exploiting. Developmental experts are still trying to figure out how malleable these constructs are, and how much of an impact they can have on future learning. For instance, do kids who hone their ANS skills become better at algebra and calculus in high school? "We still need to figure out which constructs matter most, and which are most amenable to interventions to help children improve their learning," says Star.
[O] "The hard part is, educationally, how do you build up and upon this intuitive knowledge in a way that allows a child to capture the complexity but not hold them back," says Tina Grotzer, associate professor of education at Harvard. Tapping into a child’s still developing sense of numbers and quantities is one thing, but overloading it with too many new constructs about algebra, unknowns, and problem solving may just gum up the working memory and end up adversely affecting his learning and academic performance.
[P] Still, that doesn’t mean that these innate skills shouldn’t be explored and possibly exploited in the class-room. Preschoolers may be smarter than we think, but we still have to figure out how to give them the right opportunities in the classroom so they know what to do with that knowledge.
Too many new constructs about algebra, unknowns, and problem solving may have some negative effects on a child’s learning.
选项
答案
O
解析
段末句提到,如果过多地充斥代数、未知数和问题解决方面的新构念,人的记忆功能可能就会变得混乱,并最终对孩子的学习和学业成绩产生不利影响。由此可见,过多地充斥代数、未知数和问题解决方面的新构念可能会对孩子的学业产生消极影响。本题是对该信息的转述。题中的have some negative effects对应文中的adversely affcting。
转载请注明原文地址:https://kaotiyun.com/show/Jeh7777K
0
大学英语六级
相关试题推荐
AccordingtothemostrecentAmericanFreshmansurvey,conductedannuallybytheUniversityofCalifornia,LosAngeles,undergra
Theidealcompanionmachinewouldnotonlylook,feel,andsoundfriendlybutwouldalsobeprogrammedtobehaveinanagreeable
A、Itwasself-defeating.B、Itwasaggressive.C、Itwastheessenceofcomedy.D、Itwassomethingadmirable.B事实细节题。短文中提到,现代人对幽默的
张择端是北宋时期的一位宫廷画师,拥有大量杰出作品,但最著名的应当是被后人称为“神品”的《清明上河图》(ARiverSceneatQingmingFestival)。它是一部写实主义的精品,以当时开封城的景象为题材绘制而成。图中人物多达500余人
A、Wearingarmweightswhileyouareswimming.B、Joggingvigorouslyinoneplaceforalongtime.C、Usingbicyclesthatrequirey
A、HavearestB、Haveacupofcoffee.C、Useeyedrops.D、Sleepearlyinthenight.A选项均以动词原形开头表明,本题可能考查观点建议。由对话结尾女士提到的Butthebe
中国是世界上最大的发展中国家,人口约占世界总人口的22%。在过去相当长的时期里,由于诸多原因,贫困一直困扰着中国。20世纪80年代中期,中国农村绝大多数地区凭借自身的发展优势,经济得到快速增长,但少数地区由于经济、社会、历史、自然等方面的制约,发展相对滞后
中国人的姓名有自己的传统和特点。与西方人不同,中国人的姓名是姓在前,名在后。名通常为1—2个汉字。中国人的姓名传达特殊意义,名字往往表达对新生儿的美好祝愿。有一些名字暗示出生的地点、时间或自然现象.如晨(早晨)、冬(冬天);有体现希望具有某种美德,如忠(忠
CollegeStudents’Booklist1.这是一所大学里学生所购书籍的变化2.你对于学生选择书籍类别变化的评论3.哪类书籍买得比较多?说明原因
说到中国文化,不得不提到长城。从公元前7世纪到公元16世纪,在大约2200年的时间里,先后有19个朝代修过长城,所修的长城累计有10万千米以上。三次浩大的长城修建工程,是在秦代、汉代和明代。现今存有遗迹的主要是明长城,从东边人海口的山海关(Shanhai
随机试题
在心脏,传导速度最快的是
症见汗热而黏如油,脉疾者,为
随楔状缺损加深可出现的并发症除外
甲向乙借款20万元做生意,由丙提供价值15万元的房屋抵押,并订立了抵押合同。甲因办理登记手续费过高,经乙同意未办理登记手续。甲又以自己的一辆价值6万元的"夏利"车质押给乙,双方订立了质押合同。乙认为将车放在自家附近不安全,决定仍放在甲处。一年后,甲因亏损无
设直线的方程为则直线()。
在没有正当理由的情况下对特定纳税人给予特别优惠,违背了税法基本原则中的()。
化学学习评价的根本目的是()。
Thegreatmajority______(赞成)GoldenWeekholidays.
(1)“老字号”生意每况愈下(2)出现许多仿冒“老字号”(3)仿冒付出了沉重代价(4)一个“老字号”天下闻名(5)要求保护自己的“商标权
下面程序中对一维坐标点类Point进行运算符重载#includeusingnamespacestd;classPoint{public:Point(intval){x=val;}Point
最新回复
(
0
)